Options
Fink, Bernhard
Loading...
Preferred name
Fink, Bernhard
Official Name
Fink, Bernhard
Alternative Name
Fink, B.
Main Affiliation
Now showing 1 - 10 of 19
2019Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","20180803"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","3"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Biology Letters"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","15"],["dc.contributor.author","Fink, Bernhard"],["dc.contributor.author","Butovskaya, Marina L."],["dc.contributor.author","Shackelford, Todd K."],["dc.date.accessioned","2020-12-10T18:20:16Z"],["dc.date.available","2020-12-10T18:20:16Z"],["dc.date.issued","2019"],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1098/rsbl.2018.0803"],["dc.identifier.eissn","1744-957X"],["dc.identifier.issn","1744-9561"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/75501"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.intern","DOI Import GROB-354"],["dc.title","Assessment of physical strength from gait: data from the Maasai of Tanzania"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI2019Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","e23235"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","3"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","American Journal of Human Biology"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","31"],["dc.contributor.author","Polo, Pablo"],["dc.contributor.author","Muñoz‐Reyes, Jose Antonio"],["dc.contributor.author","Pita, Miguel"],["dc.contributor.author","Shackelford, Todd K."],["dc.contributor.author","Fink, Bernhard"],["dc.date.accessioned","2021-06-01T10:49:09Z"],["dc.date.available","2021-06-01T10:49:09Z"],["dc.date.issued","2019"],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1002/ajhb.23235"],["dc.identifier.eissn","1520-6300"],["dc.identifier.issn","1042-0533"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/86183"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.intern","DOI-Import GROB-425"],["dc.relation.eissn","1520-6300"],["dc.relation.issn","1042-0533"],["dc.title","Testosterone‐dependent facial and body traits predict men's sociosexual attitudes and behaviors"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI2015Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.artnumber","e0136208"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","8"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","PLoS ONE"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","10"],["dc.contributor.author","Butovskaya, Marina L."],["dc.contributor.author","Lazebny, Oleg E."],["dc.contributor.author","Vasilyev, Vasiliy A."],["dc.contributor.author","Dronova, Daria A."],["dc.contributor.author","Karelin, Dmitri V."],["dc.contributor.author","Mabulla, Audax Z. P."],["dc.contributor.author","Shibalev, Dmitri V."],["dc.contributor.author","Shackelford, Todd K."],["dc.contributor.author","Fink, Bernhard"],["dc.contributor.author","Ryskov, Alexey P."],["dc.date.accessioned","2018-11-07T09:53:12Z"],["dc.date.available","2018-11-07T09:53:12Z"],["dc.date.issued","2015"],["dc.description.abstract","The androgen receptor (AR) gene polymorphism in humans is linked to aggression and may also be linked to reproduction. Here we report associations between AR gene polymorphism and aggression and reproduction in two small-scale societies in northern Tanzania (Africa)-the Hadza (monogamous foragers) and the Datoga (polygynous pastoralists). We secured self-reports of aggression and assessed genetic polymorphism of the number of CAG repeats for the AR gene for 210 Hadza men and 229 Datoga men (aged 17-70 years). We conducted structural equation modeling to identify links between AR gene polymorphism, aggression, and number of children born, and included age and ethnicity as covariates. Fewer AR CAG repeats predicted greater aggression, and Datoga men reported more aggression than did Hadza men. In addition, aggression mediated the identified negative relationship between CAG repeats and number of children born."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1371/journal.pone.0136208"],["dc.identifier.isi","000359919900065"],["dc.identifier.pmid","26291982"],["dc.identifier.purl","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gs-1/12088"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/36283"],["dc.notes.intern","Merged from goescholar"],["dc.notes.status","zu prüfen"],["dc.notes.submitter","Najko"],["dc.publisher","Public Library Science"],["dc.relation.issn","1932-6203"],["dc.rights","CC BY 4.0"],["dc.rights.uri","https://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0"],["dc.title","Androgen Receptor Gene Polymorphism, Aggression, and Reproduction in Tanzanian Foragers and Pastoralists"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","yes"],["dc.type.status","published"],["dc.type.version","published_version"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI PMID PMC WOS2017Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.artnumber","1427"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Frontiers in Psychology"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","8"],["dc.contributor.author","Fink, Bernhard"],["dc.contributor.author","Wübker, Marieke"],["dc.contributor.author","Ostner, Julia"],["dc.contributor.author","Butovskaya, Marina L."],["dc.contributor.author","Mezentseva, Anna"],["dc.contributor.author","Muñoz Reyes, José Antonio"],["dc.contributor.author","Sela, Yael"],["dc.contributor.author","Shackelford, Todd K."],["dc.date.accessioned","2018-02-08T08:44:51Z"],["dc.date.available","2018-02-08T08:44:51Z"],["dc.date.issued","2017"],["dc.description.abstract","Previous research documents that men and women can accurately judge male physical strength from gait, but also that the sexes differ in attractiveness judgments of strong and weak male walkers. Women's (but not men's) attractiveness assessments of strong male walkers are higher than for weak male walkers. Here, we extend this research to assessments of strong and weak male walkers in Chile, Germany, and Russia. Men and women judged videos of virtual characters, animated with the walk movements of motion-captured men, on strength and attractiveness. In two countries (Germany and Russia), these videos were additionally presented at 70% (slower) and 130% (faster) of their original speed. Stronger walkers were judged to be stronger and more attractive than weak walkers, and this effect was independent of country (but not sex). Women tended to provide higher attractiveness judgments to strong walkers, and men tended to provide higher attractiveness judgments to weak walkers. In addition, German and Russian participants rated strong walkers most attractive at slow and fast speed. Thus, across countries men and women can assess male strength from gait, although they tended to differ in attractiveness assessments of strong and weak male walkers. Attractiveness assessments of male gait may be influenced by society-specific emphasis on male physical strength."],["dc.description.sponsorship","Open-Access-Publikationsfonds 2017"],["dc.identifier.doi","10.3389/fpsyg.2017.01427"],["dc.identifier.pmid","28878720"],["dc.identifier.purl","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gs-1/14602"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/12049"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.intern","Merged from goescholar"],["dc.notes.intern","In goescholar not merged with http://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gs-1/14604 but duplicate"],["dc.notes.status","final"],["dc.publisher","Frontiers Media S.A."],["dc.relation.eissn","1664-1078"],["dc.rights","Goescholar"],["dc.rights.access","openAccess"],["dc.rights.uri","https://goescholar.uni-goettingen.de/licenses"],["dc.subject.ddc","530"],["dc.title","Cross-Cultural Investigation of Male Gait Perception in Relation to Physical Strength and Speed"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dc.type.version","published_version"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI PMID PMC2015Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","172"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Personality and Individual Differences"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","175"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","86"],["dc.contributor.author","Roeder, Susanne"],["dc.contributor.author","Weege, Bettina"],["dc.contributor.author","Carbon, Claus-Christian"],["dc.contributor.author","Shackelford, Todd K."],["dc.contributor.author","Fink, Bernhard"],["dc.date.accessioned","2018-11-07T09:49:57Z"],["dc.date.available","2018-11-07T09:49:57Z"],["dc.date.issued","2015"],["dc.description.abstract","We investigated the influence of mating context and sociosexual orientation (interest in sex without emotional involvement) on men's perceptions of women's dance movements. One hundred men aged 18 to 33 (M = 23.5, SD = 3.5) years viewed brief videos of five \"high attractive\" and five \"low attractive\" female dancers (aged 18 to 22 years; M = 19.8, SD = 1.2) from a sample of 84 motion-captured dancers, and judged them on promiscuity and movement harmony. Additionally, half the participants judged the dancers on attractiveness as a long-term mate and the other half on attractiveness as a short-term mate. Men were more attracted to high attractive dancers than to low attractive dancers and judged them higher on attractiveness when choosing as a potential short-term mate. In addition, high attractive dancers were rated higher than low attractive dancers on promiscuity and movement harmony. Specifically, promiscuity judgments predicted men's short-term attractiveness ratings, whereas movement harmony judgments predicted long-term attractiveness ratings. Men's sociosexual orientation did not influence perceptions of female dance movements. Results are discussed with reference to trade-offs in time and energy expenditure on child rearing in men's mate preferences, corroborating the hypothesis that women's body movements inform on these qualities. (C) 2015 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1016/j.paid.2015.06.020"],["dc.identifier.isi","000360255000030"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/35610"],["dc.notes.status","zu prüfen"],["dc.notes.submitter","Najko"],["dc.publisher","Pergamon-elsevier Science Ltd"],["dc.relation.issn","0191-8869"],["dc.title","Men's perception of women's dance movements depends on mating context, but not men's sociosexual orientation"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","yes"],["dc.type.status","published"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI WOS2016Conference Abstract [["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Perception"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","45"],["dc.contributor.author","Roeder, Susanne"],["dc.contributor.author","Carbon, Claus-Christian"],["dc.contributor.author","Shackelford, Todd K."],["dc.contributor.author","Pisanski, Katarzyna"],["dc.contributor.author","Weege, Bettina"],["dc.contributor.author","Fink, Bernhard"],["dc.date.accessioned","2018-11-07T10:10:24Z"],["dc.date.available","2018-11-07T10:10:24Z"],["dc.date.issued","2016"],["dc.format.extent","61"],["dc.identifier.isi","000390215900125"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/39850"],["dc.notes.status","zu prüfen"],["dc.notes.submitter","Najko"],["dc.publisher","Sage Publications Ltd"],["dc.publisher.place","London"],["dc.relation.issn","1468-4233"],["dc.relation.issn","0301-0066"],["dc.title","Men's visual attention to and perceptions of women's dance movements"],["dc.type","conference_abstract"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","yes"],["dc.type.status","published"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details WOS2014Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","138"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Personality and Individual Differences"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","141"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","58"],["dc.contributor.author","Fink, Bernhard"],["dc.contributor.author","Klappauf, Dominique"],["dc.contributor.author","Brewer, Gayle"],["dc.contributor.author","Shackelford, Todd K."],["dc.date.accessioned","2018-11-07T09:44:53Z"],["dc.date.available","2018-11-07T09:44:53Z"],["dc.date.issued","2014"],["dc.description.abstract","Women engage in intra-sexual competition to attract or to retain a mate. Given men's preferences for certain female physical characteristics, women may be attuned to potential rivals who display such traits. We examined how variation in facial femininity, breast size, and waist-to-hip ratio (WHR) affects perceived competition and attractiveness judgments in a sample of German female undergraduates. Thirty-five women ranked five images of each stimulus type according to perceived competition and rated these images for attractiveness and femininity. Women with more feminine faces, larger breasts, and lower WHRs received higher attractiveness and femininity ratings and were ranked highest on perceived competition. The results indicate the occurrence of human female intra-sexual competition with respect to physical traits desired by potential mates. (C) 2013 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1016/j.paid.2013.10.015"],["dc.identifier.isi","000328237100025"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/34493"],["dc.notes.status","zu prüfen"],["dc.notes.submitter","Najko"],["dc.publisher","Pergamon-elsevier Science Ltd"],["dc.relation.issn","0191-8869"],["dc.title","Female physical characteristics and intra-sexual competition in women"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","yes"],["dc.type.status","published"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI WOS2015Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","728"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","5"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","American Journal of Human Biology"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","730"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","27"],["dc.contributor.author","Weege, Bettina"],["dc.contributor.author","Pham, Michael N."],["dc.contributor.author","Shackelford, Todd K."],["dc.contributor.author","Fink, Bernhard"],["dc.date.accessioned","2018-11-07T09:52:51Z"],["dc.date.available","2018-11-07T09:52:51Z"],["dc.date.issued","2015"],["dc.description.abstract","ObjectivesPhysical strength provides information about male quality and can be assessed from facial and body morphology. Research on perception of dance movements indicates that body movement also provides information about male physical strength. These relationships have not been investigated for women. MethodsWe investigated relationships of handgrip strength (HGS) and dance attractiveness perception in 75 men and 84 women. ResultsWe identified positive relationships between HGS and opposite-sex assessments of dance attractiveness for men but not women. ConclusionsThe replication of previous research investigating relationships between dance attractiveness and physical strength in men corroborates the hypothesis that dance movements provide information about male quality. We argue that these relationships are interpretable in contexts of inter- and intra-sexual selection. Am. J. Hum. Biol. 27:728-730, 2015. (c) 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc."],["dc.description.sponsorship","German Science Foundation (DFG) [1450/4-1, FI 1450/7-1]"],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1002/ajhb.22703"],["dc.identifier.isi","000359677700020"],["dc.identifier.pmid","25754668"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/36211"],["dc.notes.status","zu prüfen"],["dc.notes.submitter","Najko"],["dc.publisher","Wiley-blackwell"],["dc.relation.issn","1520-6300"],["dc.relation.issn","1042-0533"],["dc.title","Physical strength and dance attractiveness: Further evidence for an association in men, but not in women"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","yes"],["dc.type.status","published"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI PMID PMC WOS2016Review [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","318"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","3"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","American Journal of Human Biology"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","329"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","28"],["dc.contributor.author","Jeffery, Austin John"],["dc.contributor.author","Pham, Michael N."],["dc.contributor.author","Shackelford, Todd K."],["dc.contributor.author","Fink, Bernhard"],["dc.date.accessioned","2018-11-07T10:14:39Z"],["dc.date.available","2018-11-07T10:14:39Z"],["dc.date.issued","2016"],["dc.description.abstract","A given man's phenotype embodies cues of his ancestral ability to effectively defend himself and his kin from harm, to survive adverse conditions, and to acquire status and mating opportunities. In this review, we explore the hypothesis that a man's phenotype also embodies cues to fertility or the probability that an ejaculate will fertilize ova. Female mate choice depends on the ability to discern the quality of a male reproductive partner through his phenotype, and male fertility may be among the traits that females have evolved to detect. A female who selects as mates males that deliver higher quality ejaculates will, on average, be more fecund than her competitors. Data on several non-human species demonstrate correlations between ejaculate quality and secondary sexual characteristics that inform female mate choice, suggesting that females may select mates in part on the basis of fertility. While the non-human literature on this topic has advanced, the human literature remains limited in scope and there is no clear consensus on appropriate methodologies or theoretical positions. We provide a comprehensive review and meta-analysis of this literature, and conclude by proposing solutions to the many issues that impede progress in the field. In the process, we hope to encourage interest and insight from investigators in other areas of human mating and reproductive biology. (C) 2015 Wiley Periodicals, Inc."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1002/ajhb.22805"],["dc.identifier.isi","000377587300002"],["dc.identifier.pmid","26626022"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/40659"],["dc.notes.status","zu prüfen"],["dc.notes.submitter","Najko"],["dc.publisher","Wiley-blackwell"],["dc.relation.issn","1520-6300"],["dc.relation.issn","1042-0533"],["dc.title","Does Human Ejaculate Quality Relate to Phenotypic Traits?"],["dc.type","review"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","yes"],["dc.type.status","published"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI PMID PMC WOS2016Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","1"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Personality and Individual Differences"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","3"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","101"],["dc.contributor.author","Roeder, Susanne"],["dc.contributor.author","Carbon, Claus-Christian"],["dc.contributor.author","Shackelford, Todd K."],["dc.contributor.author","Pisanski, Katarzyna"],["dc.contributor.author","Weege, Bettina"],["dc.contributor.author","Fink, Bernhard"],["dc.date.accessioned","2018-11-07T10:08:10Z"],["dc.date.available","2018-11-07T10:08:10Z"],["dc.date.issued","2016"],["dc.description.abstract","Sexual selectionmay have shaped male visual sensitivity to characteristics that provide information about female mate quality. Indeed, men judge certain facial and bodily configurations of women to be attractive, possibly because those configurations signal health and fertility. Most of this evidence derives from the study of women's facial and body photographs. We tested the hypothesis that attractive female dancers receive greater visual attention from men than do unattractive dancers. Twenty-nine men viewed video pairs of pre-categorized high and low attractive female dancers. Their eye gaze was tracked and they also provided ratings of attractiveness, femininity, and dance movement harmony. High attractive dancers received greater visual attention than did low attractive dancers and men's visual attention correlated positively with their judgments of attractiveness, femininity, and dance movement harmony. We discuss our findings in the context of the 'beauty captures the mind of the beholder' hypothesis and the role of dance movements in human mate selection. (C) 2016 Elsevier Ltd. All rights reserved."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1016/j.paid.2016.05.025"],["dc.identifier.isi","000383003300001"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/39420"],["dc.notes.status","zu prüfen"],["dc.notes.submitter","Najko"],["dc.publisher","Pergamon-elsevier Science Ltd"],["dc.relation.issn","0191-8869"],["dc.title","Men's visual attention to and perceptions of women's dance movements"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","yes"],["dc.type.status","published"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI WOS