Now showing 1 - 10 of 10
  • 2017Journal Article
    [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","466"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","NeuroImage"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","474"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","156"],["dc.contributor.author","Hammerschmidt, Wiebke"],["dc.contributor.author","Sennhenn-Reulen, Holger"],["dc.contributor.author","Schacht, Annekathrin"],["dc.date.accessioned","2017-09-07T11:53:44Z"],["dc.date.available","2017-09-07T11:53:44Z"],["dc.date.issued","2017"],["dc.description.abstract","Facial expressions of emotion have an undeniable processing advantage over neutral faces, discernible both at behavioral level and in emotion-related modulations of several event-related potentials (ERPs). Recently it was proposed that also inherently neutral stimuli might gain salience through associative learning mechanisms. The present study investigated whether acquired motivational salience leads to processing advantages similar to biologically determined origins of inherent emotional salience by applying an associative learning paradigm to human face processing. Participants (N=24) were trained to categorize neutral faces to salience categories by receiving different monetary outcomes. ERPs were recorded in a subsequent test phase consisting of gender decisions on previously associated faces, as well as on familiarized and novel faces expressing happy, angry or no emotion. Previously reward-associated faces boosted the P1 component, indicating that acquired reward-associations modulate early sensory processing in extrastriate visual cortex. However, ERP modulations to emotional – primarily angry – expressions expanded to subsequent processing stages, as reflected in well-established emotion-related ERPs. The present study offers new evidence that motivational salience associated to inherently neutral stimuli can sharpen sensory encoding but does not obligatorily lead to preferential processing at later stages."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1016/j.neuroimage.2017.04.032"],["dc.identifier.gro","3151351"],["dc.identifier.pmid","28416452"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/8145"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.status","final"],["dc.notes.submitter","chake"],["dc.relation.issn","1053-8119"],["dc.title","Associated motivational salience impacts early sensory processing of human faces"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","no"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]
    Details DOI PMID PMC
  • 2022Journal Article Research Paper
    [["dc.bibliographiccitation.artnumber","S1470160X22001637"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","108692"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Ecological Indicators"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","136"],["dc.contributor.author","Zeller, Laura"],["dc.contributor.author","Baumann, Charlotte"],["dc.contributor.author","Gonin, Pierre"],["dc.contributor.author","Heidrich, Lea"],["dc.contributor.author","Keye, Constanze"],["dc.contributor.author","Konrad, Felix"],["dc.contributor.author","Larrieu, Laurent"],["dc.contributor.author","Meyer, Peter"],["dc.contributor.author","Sennhenn-Reulen, Holger"],["dc.contributor.author","Müller, Jörg"],["dc.contributor.author","Ammer, Christian"],["dc.date.accessioned","2022-04-01T10:00:58Z"],["dc.date.available","2022-04-01T10:00:58Z"],["dc.date.issued","2022"],["dc.description.abstract","Effects of forest management on forest biodiversity have received increasing attention in both research and forestry practice. Despite advances in technology, monitoring of biodiversity remains time and cost-intensive and requires specific taxonomic expertise. In forest management, however, there is increasing interest and need to integrate biodiversity monitoring into forest inventories efficiently to estimate the potential effects of forest management on biodiversity. Forest management systems can differ greatly depending on management goals and the intensity and frequency of the applied silvicultural interventions. To identify management effects on biodiversity, an estimation of biodiversity using forest structural attributes may be a reasonable approach. Forest structure can – compared to conventional species-based monitoring - easily be captured during forest inventories and does not require specific taxonomic expertise. The IBP (Index of Biodiversity Potential) is a composite index aiming to provide practitioners with an efficient tool for estimating biodiversity at the local level. We recorded the IBP on 147 plots in three regions of Germany, where detailed biodiversity monitoring had been conducted. This study quantified the relationship between changes in the IBP scores and changes in species richness for 13 taxonomic groups. To determine this, we analyzed estimated relationships between the IBP and species richness using a count regression model. We found positive estimated relationships with species richness of birds, fungi, true bugs, lichens, and moths in at least 3 of 5 examined forest types. However, for spiders, bats, carabids, necrophagous and saproxylic beetles, either no relationship with the IBP or estimated relationships with only one forest type were found. Changes in scores for the IBP’s factors number of vertical layers, large living trees, tree-related microhabitats, and proportion of gaps correlated with changes in the measured species richness in many cases. Even though the IBP is generally not adequate to predict actual presence or precise number of species, it can be utilized to depict a forest stand’s potential in terms of species richness. Due to its easy and time-efficient application, it could be a useful proxy used in combination with species-based monitoring approaches."],["dc.description.sponsorship","Open-Access-Publikationsfonds 2022"],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1016/j.ecolind.2022.108692"],["dc.identifier.pii","S1470160X22001637"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/105563"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.intern","DOI-Import GROB-530"],["dc.relation.issn","1470-160X"],["dc.rights","CC BY-NC-ND 4.0"],["dc.rights.uri","https://www.elsevier.com/tdm/userlicense/1.0/"],["dc.title","Index of biodiversity potential (IBP) versus direct species monitoring in temperate forests"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dc.type.subtype","original_ja"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]
    Details DOI
  • 2016Journal Article
    [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","417"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","2"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Animal Cognition"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","428"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","19"],["dc.contributor.author","Schmitt, Vanessa"],["dc.contributor.author","Federspiel, Ira"],["dc.contributor.author","Eckert, Johanna"],["dc.contributor.author","Keupp, Stefanie"],["dc.contributor.author","Tschernek, Laura"],["dc.contributor.author","Faraut, Lauriane"],["dc.contributor.author","Schuster, Richard"],["dc.contributor.author","Michels, Corinna"],["dc.contributor.author","Sennhenn-Reulen, Holger"],["dc.contributor.author","Bugnyar, Thomas"],["dc.contributor.author","Mussweiler, Thomas"],["dc.contributor.author","Fischer, Julia"],["dc.date.accessioned","2017-09-07T11:47:09Z"],["dc.date.available","2017-09-07T11:47:09Z"],["dc.date.issued","2016"],["dc.description.abstract","Social comparisons are a fundamental characteristic of human behaviour, yet relatively little is known about their evolutionary foundations. Adapting the co-acting paradigm from human research (Seta in J Pers Soc Psychol 42:281–291, 1982. doi:10.1037//0022-3514.42.2.281, we examined how the performance of a partner influenced subjects’ performance in long-tailed macaques (Macaca fascicularis). Using parallel testing in touch screen setups in which subjects had to discriminate familiar and novel photographs of men and women, we investigated whether accuracy and reaction time were influenced by partner performance and relationship quality (affiliate vs. non-affiliate). Auditory feedback about the alleged performance of the co-actor was provided via playback; partner performance was either moderately or extremely better or worse than subject performance. We predicted that subjects would assimilate to moderately different comparison standards as well as to affiliates and contrast away from extreme standards and non-affiliates. Subjects instantly generalized to novel pictures. While accuracy was not affected by any of the factors, long reaction times occurred more frequently when subjects were tested with a non-affiliate who was performing worse, compared to one who was doing better than them (80 % quantile worse: 5.1, better: 4.3 s). For affiliate co-actors, there was no marked effect (worse: 4.4, better: 4.6 s). In a control condition with no auditory feedback, subjects performed somewhat better in the presence of affiliates (M = 77.8 % correct) compared to non-affiliates (M = 71.1 %), while reaction time was not affected. Apparently, subjects were sensitive to partner identity and performance, yet variation in motivation rather than assimilation and contrast effects may account for the observed effects."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1007/s10071-015-0943-4"],["dc.identifier.gro","3150634"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/7413"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.status","final"],["dc.relation.issn","1435-9448"],["dc.subject","Co-acting paradigm; Evolution; Inequity aversion; Meta-cognition; Monkeys; Non-human primates; Social comparison processes; Social relationships"],["dc.title","Do monkeys compare themselves to others?"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","unknown"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","no"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]
    Details DOI
  • 2018Journal Article
    [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","2131"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","10"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Methods in Ecology and Evolution"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","2144"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","9"],["dc.contributor.author","Goffe, Adeelia S."],["dc.contributor.author","Fischer, Julia"],["dc.contributor.author","Sennhenn-Reulen, Holger"],["dc.contributor.editor","Fisher, Diana"],["dc.date.accessioned","2019-07-30T07:15:26Z"],["dc.date.available","2019-07-30T07:15:26Z"],["dc.date.issued","2018"],["dc.description.abstract","1.The construction of rank hierarchies based on agonistic interactions between two individuals (“dyads”) is an important component in the characterization of the social structure of groups. To this end, winner‐loser matrices are typically created, which collapse the outcome of dyadic interactions over time, resulting in the loss of all information contained in the temporal domain. Methods that track changes in the outcome of dyadic interactions (such as “Elo‐ratings”) are receiving increasing interest. Critically, individual ratings are not just based on the succession of wins and losses, but depend on the values of start ratings and a shift coefficient. Recent studies improved existing methods by introducing a point estimation of these auxiliary parameters on the basis of a maximum likelihood (ML) approach. For a sound assessment of the rank hierarchies generated this way, we argue that measures of uncertainty of the estimates, as well as a quantification of the robustness of the methods, are also needed. 2.We introduce a Bayesian inference (BI) approach using “partial pooling”, which rests on the assumption that all start ratings are samples from the same distribution. We compare the outcome of the ML approach to that of the BI approach using real‐world data. In addition, we simulate different scenarios to explore in which way the Elo‐rating responds to social events (such as rank changes), and low numbers of observations. 3.Estimates of the start ratings based on “partial pooling” are more robust than those based on ML, also in scenarios where some individuals have only few observations. Our simulations show that assumed rank differences may fall well within the “uncertain” range, and that low sampling density, unbalanced designs, and coalitionary leaps involving several individuals within the hierarchy may yield unreliable results. 4.Our results support the view that Elo‐rating can be a powerful tool in the analysis of social behaviour, when the data meet certain criteria. Assessing the uncertainty greatly aids in the interpretation of results. We advocate running simulation approaches to test how well Elo‐ratings reflect the (simulated) true structure and how sensitive the rating is to true changes in the hierarchy. This article is protected by copyright. All rights reserved."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1111/2041-210X.13072"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/62162"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.relation.issn","2041-210X"],["dc.title","Bayesian inference and simulation approaches improve the assessment of Elo-ratings in the analysis of social behaviour"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]
    Details DOI
  • 2019Journal Article
    [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","1"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Nature Conservation"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","23"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","35"],["dc.contributor.author","Demant, Laura"],["dc.contributor.author","Meyer, Peter"],["dc.contributor.author","Sennhenn-Reulen, Holger"],["dc.contributor.author","Walentowski, Helge"],["dc.contributor.author","Bergmeier, Erwin"],["dc.date.accessioned","2019-07-09T11:51:20Z"],["dc.date.available","2019-07-09T11:51:20Z"],["dc.date.issued","2019"],["dc.description.abstract","Setting operational conservation objectives is a major challenge for effective biodiversity conservation worldwide. To analyse forest conservation objectives in Germany in a transparent manner and to achieve a consistent and consensual framework, we systematically classified conservation objectives suggested in concepts by different stakeholders. We analysed 79 biodiversity and forest conservation concepts of different stakeholder groups at various scales and applied textual content analysis and Dirichlet regression to reach a high degree of transferability and applicability. Our analysis revealed a broad consensus concerning forest conservation across stakeholders and scales, albeit with slight differences in focus, but we detected a scale-related mismatch. A wide array of conservation objectives covered social, biotic and abiotic natural resources. Conservation of species, ecosystems and structural elements in forests were found to be of primary importance across stakeholders and scale levels. Shortcomings in the conservation concepts were found in addressing genetic diversity, abiotic resources and socio-cultural objectives. Our results show that problems in forest conservation may be rooted in trade-offs between aims, targeting mismatch across scale levels and insufficient implementation of objectives."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.3897/natureconservation.35.35049"],["dc.identifier.purl","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gs-1/16109"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/59931"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.subject.ddc","570"],["dc.title","Seeking consensus in German forest conservation: An analysis of contemporary concepts"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]
    Details DOI
  • 2016Journal Article
    [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","4637"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","25"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Statistics in Medicine"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","4659"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","35"],["dc.contributor.author","Sennhenn-Reulen, Holger"],["dc.contributor.author","Kneib, Thomas"],["dc.date.accessioned","2017-09-07T11:47:45Z"],["dc.date.available","2017-09-07T11:47:45Z"],["dc.date.issued","2016"],["dc.description.abstract","Multi‐state models generalize survival or duration time analysis to the estimation of transition‐specific hazard rate functions for multiple transitions. When each of the transition‐specific risk functions is parametrized with several distinct covariate effect coefficients, this leads to a model of potentially high dimension. To decrease the parameter space dimensionality and to work out a clear image of the underlying multi‐state model structure, one can either aim at setting some coefficients to zero or to make coefficients for the same covariate but two different transitions equal. The first issue can be approached by penalizing the absolute values of the covariate coefficients as in lasso regularization. If, instead, absolute differences between coefficients of the same covariate on different transitions are penalized, this leads to sparse competing risk relations within a multi‐state model, that is, equality of covariate effect coefficients. In this paper, a new estimation approach providing sparse multi‐state modelling by the aforementioned principles is established, based on the estimation of multi‐state models and a simultaneous penalization of the L1‐norm of covariate coefficients and their differences in a structured way. The new multi‐state modelling approach is illustrated on peritoneal dialysis study data and implemented in the R package penMSM."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1002/sim.7017"],["dc.identifier.gro","3149350"],["dc.identifier.pmid","27334132"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/6017"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.intern","Kneib Crossref Import"],["dc.notes.status","final"],["dc.notes.submitter","chake"],["dc.relation.issn","0277-6715"],["dc.title","Structured fusion lasso penalized multi-state models"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","no"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]
    Details DOI PMID PMC
  • 2018Journal Article Research Paper
    [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","1107"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","9"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Language, Cognition and Neuroscience"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","1127"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","33"],["dc.contributor.author","Hosemann, Jana"],["dc.contributor.author","Herrmann, Annika"],["dc.contributor.author","Sennhenn-Reulen, Holger"],["dc.contributor.author","Schlesewsky, Matthias"],["dc.contributor.author","Steinbach, Markus"],["dc.date.accessioned","2019-07-25T15:43:42Z"],["dc.date.available","2019-07-25T15:43:42Z"],["dc.date.issued","2018"],["dc.description.abstract","Previous studies on agreement violation in sign languages report neurophysiological responses similar to those observed for spoken languages. In contrast, the two current event-related potential studies (ERP) on agreement violations in German Sign Language sentences present results that allow for an alternative explanation. In experiment A, we investigated the processing of agreement verbs ending in an unspecified location different to the location associated with the referent. Incorrect agreement verbs engendered a posterior positivity effect (220–570 ms post nonmanual cues) and a left anterior effect (300–600 ms post the subsequent sign onset). In experiment B, we investigated a violation of morphologically modified plain verbs. Incorrect plain verbs, articulated to express third person object agreement, engendered a broadly distributed positivity effect (420–730 ms post mismatch onset). We discuss the results under the perspective of enhanced costs for context updating, and argue that sign language agreement is based on phonological and pragmatic principles."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1080/23273798.2018.1465986"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/62071"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.relation.issn","2327-3798"],["dc.relation.issn","2327-3801"],["dc.relation.orgunit","Abteilung Germanistische Linguistik"],["dc.title","Agreement or no agreement. ERP correlates of verb agreement violation in German Sign Language"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dc.type.subtype","original_ja"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]
    Details DOI
  • 2017Journal Article
    [["dc.bibliographiccitation.artnumber","e22711"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","11"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","American Journal of Primatology"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","79"],["dc.contributor.author","Almeling, Laura"],["dc.contributor.author","Sennhenn-Reulen, Holger"],["dc.contributor.author","Hammerschmidt, Kurt"],["dc.contributor.author","Freund, Alexandra M."],["dc.contributor.author","Fischer, Julia"],["dc.date.accessioned","2018-03-08T09:21:20Z"],["dc.date.available","2018-03-08T09:21:20Z"],["dc.date.issued","2017"],["dc.description.abstract","Human aging is accompanied by a decrease in social activity and a narrowing in social networks. Studies in nonhuman primates may provide valuable comparative insights in which way aging impacts social life, in the absence of cultural conventions and an awareness of a limited lifetime. For female Barbary macaques at “La Forêt des Singes” in Rocamadour, France, we previously reported an age-associated decrease in active grooming time and network size. Here, we aimed to extend these findings by investigating in which way physical decline, spatial proximity, and aggression vary with age in female Barbary macaques. We analyzed >1,200 hr of focal observations for 46 females aged 5–29 years. As expected, older females engaged less frequently in challenging locomotor activity, such as climbing or running, than younger ones. The previously reported decrease in grooming time was not due to shorter grooming bout duration. Instead, active grooming bouts lasted even longer, which discounts the idea that manual fatigue explains the shift in grooming pattern. We found that older females tended to be spatially reclusive and that they were less frequently the targets of aggression. Although older females showed aggressive behaviors at similar rates as younger females, the proportion of low-level aggression (i.e., threats) increased with age. We suggest that these threats are not simply a signal of dominance, but also function to deter approaches by others. Overall, these findings are in line with the idea that older females aim to avoid potentially negative interactions, specifically if these are costly. In sum, these findings support the idea that shifts in female Barbary macaques' grooming activity, do not simply result from physical deterioration, but are instead due to a higher selectivity in the choice of social partners."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1002/ajp.22711"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/12847"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.intern","GRO-Li-Import"],["dc.notes.status","final"],["dc.relation.doi","10.1002/ajp.22711"],["dc.relation.issn","0275-2565"],["dc.title","Social interactions and activity patterns of old Barbary macaques: Further insights into the foundations of social selectivity"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","unknown"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]
    Details DOI
  • 2017Journal Article
    [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","57"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Animal Behaviour"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","66"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","130"],["dc.contributor.author","Fischer, Julia"],["dc.contributor.author","Farnworth, Max S."],["dc.contributor.author","Sennhenn-Reulen, Holger"],["dc.contributor.author","Hammerschmidt, Kurt"],["dc.date.accessioned","2018-03-08T09:21:20Z"],["dc.date.available","2018-03-08T09:21:20Z"],["dc.date.issued","2017"],["dc.description.abstract","Social complexity has been invoked as a driving force shaping communicative and cognitive abilities, and brain evolution more generally. Despite progress in the conceptual understanding of societal structures, there is still a dearth of quantitative measures to capture social complexity. Here we offer a method to quantify social complexity in terms of the diversity of differentiated relationships. We illustrate our approach using data collected from Barbary macaques, Macaca sylvanus, at ‘La Forêt des Singes’ in Rocamadour, France, as well as simulated data sets for a proof-of-concept. Based on affiliative and agonistic behavioural categories, we calculated four indices that characterize social relationships (diversity of behavioural patterns, dyadic composite sociality index, relative interaction frequency and tenor). Using cluster analyses, we identified four different relationship types: rarely interacting agonistic dyads, rarely interacting affiliative dyads, moderately frequently interacting ambivalent dyads and frequently interacting affiliative dyads. We then calculated for each individual a derived diversity score that integrates information about the number and diversity of relationships each subject maintained. At the individual level, one may be interested to identify predictors of this individual diversity score, such as age, rank or sex. At the group level, variation in the relative shares of affiliative and agonistic interactions affects the distribution of individual diversity scores more than the interaction frequency, while the omission of ambivalent relationships (i.e. a discontinuous variation in the share of affiliative or agonistic relationships) leads to greater variation in diversity scores. The number of realized relationships had only a moderate effect. Overall, this method appears to be suited to capture social complexity in terms of the diversity of relationships at the individual and group level. We suggest that this approach is applicable across different species and facilitates quantitative tests of putative drivers in brain evolution."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.06.003"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/12846"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.intern","GRO-Li-Import"],["dc.notes.status","final"],["dc.relation.doi","10.1016/j.anbehav.2017.06.003"],["dc.relation.issn","0003-3472"],["dc.title","Quantifying social complexity"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","unknown"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]
    Details DOI
  • 2016Journal Article
    [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","1744"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","13"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Current Biology"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","1749"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","26"],["dc.contributor.author","Almeling, Laura"],["dc.contributor.author","Hammerschmidt, Kurt"],["dc.contributor.author","Sennhenn-Reulen, Holger"],["dc.contributor.author","Freund, Alexandra M."],["dc.contributor.author","Fischer, Julia"],["dc.date.accessioned","2017-09-07T11:47:15Z"],["dc.date.available","2017-09-07T11:47:15Z"],["dc.date.issued","2016"],["dc.description.abstract","As humans age, they become more selective regarding their personal goals [1] and social partners [2]. Whereas the selectivity in goals has been attributed to losses in resources (e.g., physical strength) [3], the increasing focus on emotionally meaningful partners is, according to socioemotional selectivity theory, driven by the awareness of one's decreasing future lifetime [2]. Similar to humans, aging monkeys show physical losses [4] and reductions in social activity [2, 5-7]. To disentangle a general resource loss and the awareness of decreasing time, we combined field experiments with behavioral observations in a large age-heterogeneous population of Barbary macaques (Macaca sylvanus) at La Forêt des Singes. Novel object tests revealed a loss of interest in the nonsocial environment in early adulthood, which was modulated by the availability of a food reward. Experiments using vocal and visual representations of social partners indicated that monkeys maintained an interest in social stimuli and a preferential interest in friends and socially important individuals into old age. Old females engaged in fewer social interactions, although other group members continued to invest in relationships with them. Consequently, reductions in sociality were not due to a decrease in social interest. In conclusion, some of the motivational shifts observed in aging humans, particularly the increasing focus on social over nonsocial stimuli, may occur in the absence of a limited time perspective and are most likely deeply rooted in primate evolution. Our findings highlight the value of nonhuman primates as valuable models for understanding human aging [8, 9]."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1016/j.cub.2016.04.066"],["dc.identifier.gro","3150649"],["dc.identifier.pmid","27345168"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/7428"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.status","final"],["dc.relation.issn","0960-9822"],["dc.title","Motivational Shifts in Aging Monkeys and the Origins of Social Selectivity"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","unknown"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","no"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]
    Details DOI PMID PMC