Options
Mojzisch, Andreas
Loading...
Preferred name
Mojzisch, Andreas
Official Name
Mojzisch, Andreas
Alternative Name
Mojzisch, A.
Now showing 1 - 4 of 4
2008Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","1477"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","6"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Journal of Experimental Social Psychology"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","1490"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","44"],["dc.contributor.author","Mojzisch, Andreas"],["dc.contributor.author","Schulz-Hardt, Stefan"],["dc.contributor.author","Kerschreiter, Rudolf"],["dc.contributor.author","Brodbeck, Felix C."],["dc.contributor.author","Frey, Dieter"],["dc.date.accessioned","2017-09-07T11:54:51Z"],["dc.date.available","2017-09-07T11:54:51Z"],["dc.date.issued","2008"],["dc.description.abstract","Shared information has a stronger impact on group decisions than unshared information. A prominent explanation for this phenomenon is that shared information can be socially validated during group discussion and, hence, is perceived as more accurate and relevant than unshared information. In the present study we argue that this explanation only holds for preference-inconsistent information (i.e., information contradicting the group members’ initial preferences) but not for preference-consistent information. In Experiments 1 and 2 participants studied the protocol of a fictitious group discussion. In this protocol, we manipulated which types of information were socially validated. As predicted, social validation increased the decisional impact of preference-inconsistent but not preference-consistent information. In both experiments the effect of social validation was mediated by the perceived quality of information. Experiment 3 replicated the results of the first two experiments in an interactive setting in which two confederates discussed a decision case face-to-face with one participant."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1016/j.jesp.2008.07.012"],["dc.identifier.gro","3151514"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/8319"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.status","final"],["dc.notes.submitter","chake"],["dc.relation.issn","0022-1031"],["dc.title","Social validation in group decision-making: Differential effects on the decisional impact of preference-consistent and preference-inconsistent information"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","no"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI2008Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","679"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","5"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","691"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","34"],["dc.contributor.author","Kerschreiter, Rudolf"],["dc.contributor.author","Schulz-Hardt, Stefan"],["dc.contributor.author","Mojzisch, Andreas"],["dc.contributor.author","Frey, Dieter"],["dc.date.accessioned","2018-11-07T11:15:47Z"],["dc.date.available","2018-11-07T11:15:47Z"],["dc.date.issued","2008"],["dc.description.abstract","When searching for information, groups that are homogeneous regarding their members' prediscussion decision preferences show a strong bias for information that supports rather than conflicts with the prevailing opinion (confirmation bias). The present research examined whether homogeneous groups blindly search for information confirming their beliefs irrespective of the anticipated task or whether they are sensitive to the usefulness of new information for this forthcoming task. Results of three experiments show that task sensitivity depends on the groups' confidence in the correctness of their decision: Moderately confident groups displayed a strong confirmation bias when they anticipated having to give reasons for their decision but showed a balanced information search or even a disconfirmation bias (i.e., predominately seeking conflicting information) when they anticipated having to refute counterarguments. In contrast, highly confident groups demonstrated a strong confirmation bias independent of the anticipated task requirements."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1177/0146167207313934"],["dc.identifier.isi","000255167900008"],["dc.identifier.pmid","18310314"],["dc.identifier.purl","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gs-1/13092"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/54442"],["dc.notes.intern","Merged from goescholar"],["dc.notes.status","zu prüfen"],["dc.notes.submitter","Najko"],["dc.publisher","Sage Publications Inc"],["dc.relation.issn","0146-1672"],["dc.rights","Goescholar"],["dc.rights.uri","https://goescholar.uni-goettingen.de/licenses"],["dc.title","Biased information search in homogeneous groups: Confidence as a moderator for the effect of anticipated task requirements"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","yes"],["dc.type.status","published"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI PMID PMC WOS2006Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","1080"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","6"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Journal of Personality and Social Psychology"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","1093"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","91"],["dc.contributor.author","Schulz-Hardt, Stefan"],["dc.contributor.author","Brodbeck, Felix C."],["dc.contributor.author","Mojzisch, Andreas"],["dc.contributor.author","Kerschreiter, Rudolf"],["dc.contributor.author","Frey, Dieter"],["dc.date.accessioned","2017-09-07T11:54:50Z"],["dc.date.available","2017-09-07T11:54:50Z"],["dc.date.issued","2006"],["dc.description.abstract","The effect of diversity in individual prediscussion preferences on group decision quality was examined in an experiment in which 135 three-person groups worked on a personnel selection case with 4 alternatives. The information distribution among group members constituted a hidden profile (i.e., the correct solution was not identifiable on the basis of the members' individual information and could be detected only by pooling and integrating the members' unique information). Whereas groups with homogeneous suboptimal prediscussion preferences (no dissent) hardly ever solved the hidden profile, solution rates were significantly higher in groups with prediscussion dissent, even if none of these individual prediscussion preferences were correct. If dissent came from a proponent of the correct solution, solution rates were even higher than in dissent groups without such a proponent. The magnitude of dissent (i.e., minority dissent or full diversity of individual preferences) did not affect decision quality. The beneficial effect of dissent on group decision quality was mediated primarily by greater discussion intensity and to some extent also by less discussion bias in dissent groups."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1037/0022-3514.91.6.1080"],["dc.identifier.gro","3151523"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/8328"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.status","final"],["dc.notes.submitter","chake"],["dc.relation.issn","1939-1315"],["dc.title","Group decision making in hidden profile situations: Dissent as a facilitator for decision quality"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","unknown"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","no"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI2008Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","203"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","2"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Small Group Research"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","223"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","39"],["dc.contributor.author","Mojzisch, Andreas"],["dc.contributor.author","Schulz-Hardt, Stefan"],["dc.contributor.author","Kerschreiter, Rudolf"],["dc.contributor.author","Frey, Dieter"],["dc.date.accessioned","2017-09-07T11:54:48Z"],["dc.date.available","2017-09-07T11:54:48Z"],["dc.date.issued","2008"],["dc.description.abstract","There is conclusive evidence that information search processes are typically biased in favor of the information seeker's own opinion (confirmation bias). Less is known about how knowledge about others' opinions affects this confirmatory information search. In the present study, the authors manipulated feedback about others' opinions and anticipation of group interaction. As predicted, the effect of knowledge about others' opinions on confirmatory information search depended on whether participants anticipated interacting with these others. Specifically, minority members anticipating a group discussion exhibited a particularly strong confirmation bias, whereas minority members who did not anticipate a discussion predominantly sought information opposing their opinion. For participants not anticipating group interaction, confidence about the correctness of one's decision mediated the impact of knowledge about others' opinions on confirmatory information search. Results are discussed with regard to the debiasing effect of preference heterogeneity on confirmatory information search in groups."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1177/1046496408315983"],["dc.identifier.gro","3151512"],["dc.identifier.purl","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gs-1/13017"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/8316"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.intern","Merged from goescholar"],["dc.notes.status","public"],["dc.notes.submitter","chake"],["dc.relation.issn","1046-4964"],["dc.rights","Goescholar"],["dc.rights.uri","https://goescholar.uni-goettingen.de/licenses"],["dc.title","Combined Effects of Knowledge About Others' Opinions and Anticipation of Group Discussion on Confirmatory Information Search"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","unknown"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","no"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI