Options
Dammhahn, Melanie
Loading...
Preferred name
Dammhahn, Melanie
Official Name
Dammhahn, Melanie
Alternative Name
Dammhahn, M.
Now showing 1 - 10 of 26
2019Journal Article Research Paper [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","1234"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","8"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Methods in Ecology and Evolution"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","1245"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","10"],["dc.contributor.author","Schlägel, Ulrike E."],["dc.contributor.author","Signer, Johannes"],["dc.contributor.author","Herde, Antje"],["dc.contributor.author","Eden, Sophie"],["dc.contributor.author","Jeltsch, Florian"],["dc.contributor.author","Eccard, Jana A."],["dc.contributor.author","Dammhahn, Melanie"],["dc.contributor.editor","Murrell, David"],["dc.date.accessioned","2021-12-08T12:27:43Z"],["dc.date.available","2021-12-08T12:27:43Z"],["dc.date.issued","2019"],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1111/2041-210X.13235"],["dc.identifier.eissn","2041-210X"],["dc.identifier.issn","2041-210X"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/95428"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.intern","DOI-Import GROB-476"],["dc.relation.eissn","2041-210X"],["dc.relation.issn","2041-210X"],["dc.relation.orgunit","Abteilung Wildtierwissenschaften"],["dc.title","Estimating interactions between individuals from concurrent animal movements"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dc.type.subtype","original_ja"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"],["local.message.claim","2021-10-06T08:18:57.112+0000|||rp114797|||submit_approve|||dc_contributor_author|||None"]]Details DOI2012Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","513"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","2"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","International Journal of Primatology"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","513"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","33"],["dc.contributor.author","Dammhahn, Melanie"],["dc.date.accessioned","2022-10-06T13:32:31Z"],["dc.date.available","2022-10-06T13:32:31Z"],["dc.date.issued","2012"],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1007/s10764-012-9597-0"],["dc.identifier.pii","9597"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/115389"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.intern","DOI-Import GROB-602"],["dc.relation.eissn","1573-8604"],["dc.relation.issn","0164-0291"],["dc.relation.orgunit","Deutsches Primatenzentrum"],["dc.title","Erratum to: Anne M. Burrows and Leanne T. Nash (Eds.): The Evolution of Exudativory in Primates. Developments in Primatology: Progress and Prospects (Series Editor R. H. Tuttle)"],["dc.title.alternative","Springer, New York, 2010, xxii + 303 pp. ISBN 978-1-4419-6660-5 (USD $179.00/EUR 129.95, Hardcover)"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","unknown"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI2012Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","21"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","1"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Behavioral Ecology"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","28"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","24"],["dc.contributor.author","Lührs, Mia-Lana"],["dc.contributor.author","Dammhahn, Melanie"],["dc.contributor.author","Kappeler, Peter"],["dc.date.accessioned","2017-09-07T11:48:32Z"],["dc.date.available","2017-09-07T11:48:32Z"],["dc.date.issued","2012"],["dc.description.abstract","Group-living has been recognized as one of the major transitions in evolution. Male sociality along with solitary females is rare in mammals, but it can provide unique insights into the evolution of sociality and cooperation. Because males compete with each other over females, male cooperation in mammals has been explained by joint defense of females against other males. Here, we demonstrate that the benefits of male cooperative hunting can play a major role in shaping sociality. By quantifying differences in morphology, activity, diet, and mating success, we show that in Madagascar’s top predator, the fosa ( Cryptoprocta ferox ), some males associate to jointly hunt large prey, which allows them to grow bigger than both solitary males and females. These associated males’ physical superiority also represents an advantage in contest competition for females, as reflected by higher mating success. Our results demonstrate that enhanced access to food resources by cooperative hunting is a key to physical development and competitiveness in fosas. In contrast to previous findings, we show that male sociality must not be limited to joint defense of territory and females, but that cooperation in food acquisition can favor sociality in sexually dimorphic species."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1093/beheco/ars150"],["dc.identifier.gro","3150843"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/7637"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.status","final"],["dc.relation.issn","1465-7279"],["dc.subject","cooperative hunting; Cryptoprocta ferox; evolution of group-living; fosa; Madagascar mongoose; male sociality; male–male competition"],["dc.title","Strength in numbers: males in a carnivore grow bigger when they associate and hunt cooperatively"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","unknown"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","no"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI2015Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","54"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Experimental Gerontology"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","61"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","61"],["dc.contributor.author","Hamalainen, Anni"],["dc.contributor.author","Dammhahn, Melanie"],["dc.contributor.author","Aujard, Fabienne"],["dc.contributor.author","Kraus, Cornelia"],["dc.date.accessioned","2018-11-07T10:03:59Z"],["dc.date.available","2018-11-07T10:03:59Z"],["dc.date.issued","2015"],["dc.description.abstract","Muscle strength reflects physical functioning, declines at old age and predicts health and survival in humans and laboratory animals. Age-associated muscle deterioration causes loss of strength and may impair fitness of wild animals. However, the effects of age and life-history characteristics on muscle strength in wild animals are unknown. We investigated environment-and sex-specific patterns of physical functioning by measuring grip strength in wild and captive gray mouse lemurs. We expected more pronounced strength senescence in captivity due to condition-dependent, extrinsic mortality found in nature. Males were predicted to be stronger but potentially experience more severe senescence than females as predicted by life history theory. We found similar senescent declines in captive males and females as well as wild females, whereas wild males showed little decline, presumably due to their early mortality. Captive animals were generally weaker and showed earlier declines than wild animals. Unexpectedly, females tended to be stronger than males, especially in the reproductive season. Universal intrinsic mechanisms (e. g. sarcopenia) likely cause the similar patterns of strength loss across settings. The female advantage in muscle strength merits further study; it may follow higher reproductive investment by males, or be an adaptation associated with female social dominance. (C) 2014 Elsevier Inc. All rights reserved."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1016/j.exger.2014.11.017"],["dc.identifier.isi","000347468500008"],["dc.identifier.pmid","25446501"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/38595"],["dc.notes.status","zu prüfen"],["dc.notes.submitter","Najko"],["dc.publisher","Pergamon-elsevier Science Ltd"],["dc.relation.issn","1873-6815"],["dc.relation.issn","0531-5565"],["dc.title","Losing grip: Senescent decline in physical strength in a small-bodied primate in captivity and in the wild"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","yes"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","yes"],["dc.type.status","published"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI PMID PMC WOS2008Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","473"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","3"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Oecologia"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","483"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","157"],["dc.contributor.author","Dammhahn, Melanie"],["dc.contributor.author","Kappeler, Peter"],["dc.date.accessioned","2017-09-07T11:48:22Z"],["dc.date.available","2017-09-07T11:48:22Z"],["dc.date.issued","2008"],["dc.description.abstract","Understanding the co-occurrence of ecologically similar species remains a puzzling issue in community ecology. The species-rich mouse lemurs (Microcebus spec.) are distributed over nearly all remaining forest areas of Madagascar with a high variability in species distribution patterns. Locally, many congeneric species pairs seem to co-occur, but only little detailed information on spatial patterns is available. Here, we present the results of an intensive capture–mark–recapture study of sympatric Microcebus berthae and M. murinus populations that revealed small-scale mutual spatial exclusion. Nearest neighbour analysis indicated a spatial aggregation in Microcebus murinus but not in M. berthae. Although the diet of both species differed in proportions of food categories, they used the same food sources and had high feeding niche overlap. Also, forest structure related to the spatial distribution of main food sources did not explain spatial segregation because parts used by each species exclusively did not differ in density of trees, dead wood and lianas. We propose that life history trade-offs that result in species aggregation and a relative increase in the strength of intra-specific over inter-specific competition best explain the observed pattern of co-occurrence of ecologically similar congeneric Microcebus species."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1007/s00442-008-1079-x"],["dc.identifier.gro","3150794"],["dc.identifier.pmid","18574599"],["dc.identifier.purl","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?goescholar/3065"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/7585"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.intern","Merged from goescholar"],["dc.notes.status","final"],["dc.relation.issn","0029-8549"],["dc.rights","Goescholar"],["dc.rights.uri","https://goescholar.uni-goettingen.de/licenses"],["dc.title","Small-scale coexistence of two mouse lemur species (Microcebus berthae and M. murinus) within a homogeneous competitive environment"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","unknown"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","yes"],["dc.type.version","published_version"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI PMID PMC2012Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","1131"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","5"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Animal Behaviour"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","1139"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","84"],["dc.contributor.author","Dammhahn, Melanie"],["dc.contributor.author","Almeling, Laura"],["dc.date.accessioned","2022-10-06T13:32:55Z"],["dc.date.available","2022-10-06T13:32:55Z"],["dc.date.issued","2012"],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1016/j.anbehav.2012.08.014"],["dc.identifier.pii","S0003347212003636"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/115495"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.intern","DOI-Import GROB-602"],["dc.relation.issn","0003-3472"],["dc.relation.orgunit","Deutsches Primatenzentrum"],["dc.title","Is risk taking during foraging a personality trait? A field test for cross-context consistency in boldness"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","unknown"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI2013Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","249"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","2"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","American Journal of Physical Anthropology"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","259"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","153"],["dc.contributor.author","Dammhahn, Melanie"],["dc.contributor.author","Kappeler, Peter"],["dc.date.accessioned","2017-09-07T11:48:36Z"],["dc.date.available","2017-09-07T11:48:36Z"],["dc.date.issued","2013"],["dc.description.abstract","Understanding the mechanisms maintaining local species richness is a major topic in tropical ecology. In ecological communities of Madagascar, primates represent a major part of mammalian diversity and, thus, are a suitable taxon to study these mechanisms. Previous research suggested that ecological niche differentiation facilitates the coexistence of lemurs. However, detailed data on all species making up diverse local primate assemblages is rarely available, hampering community-wide tests of niche differentiation among Malagasy mammals. Here, we took an indirect approach and used stable isotopes as long-term indicators of individuals' diets to answer the question of whether trophic patterns and food-related mechanisms stabilize coexistence in a species-rich lemur community. We analyzed stable carbon and nitrogen isotopes in hair collected from eight syntopic lemurs in Kirindy Forest. We found that lemur species were well separated into trophic niches and ranged over two trophic levels. Furthermore, species were densely packed in isotopic space suggesting that past competitive interactions between species are a major structuring force of this dry forest lemur community. Results of other comparative studies on primates and our findings underline that—in contrast to communities worldwide—the structure and composition of lemur communities follow predictions of ecological niche theory. Patterns of competitive interactions might be more clearly revealed in Malagasy primate communities than elsewhere because lemurs represent a large fraction of ecologically interacting species in these communities. The pronounced trophic niche differentiation among lemurs is most likely due to intense competition in the past as is characteristic for adaptive radiations."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1002/ajpa.22426"],["dc.identifier.gro","3150872"],["dc.identifier.pmid","24242869"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/7667"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.status","final"],["dc.relation.issn","0002-9483"],["dc.title","Stable isotope analyses reveal dense trophic species packing and clear niche differentiation in a malagasy primate community"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","unknown"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","yes"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI PMID PMC2014Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","35"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Malagasy Nature"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","48"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","8"],["dc.contributor.author","Dittmann, Marie Theres"],["dc.contributor.author","Dammhahn, Melanie"],["dc.contributor.author","Kappeler, Peter"],["dc.date.accessioned","2018-03-20T13:49:09Z"],["dc.date.available","2018-03-20T13:49:09Z"],["dc.date.issued","2014"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/13102"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.status","final"],["dc.title","The impact of selective logging on behavior and ecology of Aphaenogaster swammerdami (Formicidae): 20 years later – a happy ant?"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","unknown"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details2013Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","793"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","3"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Functional Ecology"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","799"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","27"],["dc.contributor.author","Vuarin, Pauline"],["dc.contributor.author","Dammhahn, Melanie"],["dc.contributor.author","Henry, Pierre‐Yves"],["dc.contributor.editor","Piersma, Theunis"],["dc.date.accessioned","2022-10-06T13:24:57Z"],["dc.date.available","2022-10-06T13:24:57Z"],["dc.date.issued","2013"],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1111/1365-2435.12069"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/114713"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.intern","DOI-Import GROB-602"],["dc.relation.eissn","1365-2435"],["dc.relation.issn","0269-8463"],["dc.relation.orgunit","Deutsches Primatenzentrum"],["dc.title","Individual flexibility in energy saving: body size and condition constrain torpor use"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","unknown"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI2009Journal Article [["dc.bibliographiccitation.firstpage","599"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.issue","4"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.journal","Animal Cognition"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.lastpage","609"],["dc.bibliographiccitation.volume","12"],["dc.contributor.author","Lührs, Mia-Lana"],["dc.contributor.author","Dammhahn, Melanie"],["dc.contributor.author","Kappeler, Peter"],["dc.contributor.author","Fichtel, Claudia"],["dc.date.accessioned","2017-09-07T11:48:34Z"],["dc.date.available","2017-09-07T11:48:34Z"],["dc.date.issued","2009"],["dc.description.abstract","Wild animals face the challenge of locating feeding sites distributed across broad spatial and temporal scales. Spatial memory allows animals to find a goal, such as a productive feeding patch, even when there are no goal-specific sensory cues available. Because there is little experimental information on learning and memory capabilities in free-ranging primates, the aim of this study was to test whether grey mouse lemurs (Microcebus murinus), as short-term dietary specialists, rely on spatial memory in relocating productive feeding sites. In addition, we asked what kind of spatial representation might underlie their orientation in their natural environment. Using an experimental approach, we set eight radio-collared grey mouse lemurs a memory task by confronting them with two different spatial patterns of baited and non-baited artificial feeding stations under exclusion of sensory cues. Positional data were recorded by focal animal observations within a grid system of small foot trails. A change in the baiting pattern revealed that grey mouse lemurs primarily used spatial cues to relocate baited feeding stations and that they were able to rapidly learn a new spatial arrangement. Spatially concentrated, non-random movements revealed preliminary evidence for a route-based restriction in mouse lemur space; during a subsequent release experiment, however, we found high travel efficiency in directed movements. We therefore propose that mouse lemur spatial memory is based on some kind of mental representation that is more detailed than a route-based network map."],["dc.identifier.doi","10.1007/s10071-009-0219-y"],["dc.identifier.gro","3150855"],["dc.identifier.pmid","19263100"],["dc.identifier.purl","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?goescholar/3567"],["dc.identifier.uri","https://resolver.sub.uni-goettingen.de/purl?gro-2/7649"],["dc.language.iso","en"],["dc.notes.intern","Merged from goescholar"],["dc.notes.status","final"],["dc.relation.issn","1435-9448"],["dc.rights","Goescholar"],["dc.rights.uri","https://goescholar.uni-goettingen.de/licenses"],["dc.subject","Spatial cognition; Topological map; Euclidean map; Correlated random walk; Goal-directed movement; Microcebus murinus"],["dc.title","Spatial memory in the grey mouse lemur (Microcebus murinus)"],["dc.type","journal_article"],["dc.type.internalPublication","unknown"],["dc.type.peerReviewed","no"],["dc.type.version","published_version"],["dspace.entity.type","Publication"]]Details DOI PMID PMC
- «
- 1 (current)
- 2
- 3
- »